28.05.2018

Methods of teaching mentally retarded children

administrator No comments

What happens to a child’s social and intellectual development when they methods of teaching mentally retarded children not have the opportunity to form an attachment to a caregiver? Can the negative effects of privation be reversed?

Genie was discovered at the age of 13 when her mother, supposedly mistakenly, took her to a social services office. When she was discovered she was severely undernourished, could not stand properly or walk normally, could not understand any language, and could not speak. Genie had spent the first years of her life imprisoned alone in her bedroom by her father in order to ‘protect’ her as she was ‘mentally retarded’. Genie was a case study the afforded psychologists the overwhelming benefit of providing a unique case to study in extremely rich detail, however it must be remembered that she was just one unique individual and so the findings cannot necessarily be generalised to the whole population. A unique opportunity to study a unique individual in great detail.

A unique case that may not be generalisable to the whole population. It is not known what underlying abnormalities Genie may have had when she was born. Her father stated that she was ‘mentally retarded’, but we only have his word for that. We can never know, therefore, if Genie failed to develop skills in all areas including language because of her privation or because she was born with an underlying learning difficulty. Genie was studied at length by the researchers who adopted her, and she formed a degree of attachment to them.

However it could be argued that they treated her unethically by using her as a research subject instead of simply giving her the love and caring she needed. It is possible that the researchers had their own agendas that were not completely in Genie’s best interests. Can the effects of privation be reversed? Koluchova reported a case study of identical Czechoslovakian twins who were discovered at the age of 7 years. They had been kept locked in isolation in a cellar with only each other for company, and had been mistreated and beaten, and so they had not formed any attachment with any adult caregiver. When they were discovered they had very little speech and communicated mostly in gestures.

After they were discovered they were fostered by two sisters who gave them a loving home. It seems therefore that, given the right sort of loving and caring environment and the opportunity to develop an attachment to a sensitive caregiver, the effects of privation may be reversible. The twins were discovered at the age of 7 which may explain why they recovered from the effects of privation where Genie, who was discovered at the age of 13, did not. The twins were not completely isolated as they had each other. Being able to form an attachment to another person, in this case the other twin rather than a caregiver, may have protected them from some of the negative effects of privation. Although the twins developed to be described as ‘above average’, it is not known how well they may have developed had they not suffered privation, and so it is not known exactly how well the negative effects were reversed. Again, this is a case study of unique individuals and so the findings may not be generalisable to the whole population.

Children who live in large children’s homes do not necessarily have the same opportunity to form an attachment to a single primary caregiver as children in normal family homes. Whilst this is not as severe as privation, the lack of a sensitive primary caregiver still has negative effects on social and intellectual development Several studies have looked at the extent of the negative effects of institutionalisation and whether or not the effects can be reversed. 65 children who had been placed in a children’s home when they were less than 4 months old. The children’s home had a policy forbidding the staff to form attachments with the children, and so the care given was functional and lacked warmth.

By the age of 4 years, 24 of the children had been adopted by foster parents, 15 had been returned to their natural families, and the rest remained in the home. The findings of the study were that the adopted children fared better than the ‘restored’ children in that they tended to form closer attachments to their adopted parents than the ‘restored’ children did to their natural parents. However, both groups of children were less successful than the control children at forming peer relationships, and both groups also tended to seek far more adult attention and approval than the control children did. Tizard ignores the underlying temperament of the children involved.

It may well be that the children selected for adoption were more emotionally stable and sociable than the ones reunited with their natural parents, and this individual difference in temperament could account for them forming closer relationships with their adopted parents. In the 1980s and 1990s many children were discovered in overcrowded Romanian orphanages with very little care provided by the staff running the institutions. The children were fed, clothed and kept warm, but the vast majority had never experienced any form of sensitive care on an emotional level. British families, some before the age of 6 months and some older than 6 months. What has research told us about the effects of privation of attachment?

6 AO1 marks come from defining privation and describing the research. Case studies are useful but you should also include research into institutionalised children. Using all 4 key studies on this page will show depth to your knowledge. 6 AO2 marks come from evaluating the research and drawing a conclusion.